|
Table of Contents
Cultural Diffusion: Baseball in JapanBaseball began in Japan in 1867 when a visiting professor from the US, Horace Wilson, taught his students at Kaisei School (now the University of Tokyo) how to play the game. The first formal game was put on in 1873 by another teacher from the United States. The popularity of the sport skyrocketed in 1896 after newspapers reported an unprecedented event: Japanese culture was receptive to baseball. The Japanese found the one-on-one battle between pitcher and batter psychologically similar to sumo and martial arts. Another visit by Americans — a 1934 tour by Babe Ruth and other baseball stars — led to the formation of professional baseball leagues in Japan. Today, baseball (or, as the Japanese call it, besuboru) is the country’s dominant spectator sport. Surveys indicate that one out of every two Japanese is a baseball fan, including the emperor. Each year, professional baseball attracts 12 million spectators and huge television audiences. Japan’s oldest and most successful team, the Tokyo Yomiuri Giants, draws standing-room-only crowds throughout its 130-game season. Japanese baseball is an excellent example of cultural diffusion. While the structure of the game is similar to that of American baseball, the climate and texture of Japanese baseball have been deeply influenced by Japanese cultural values, such as self-discipline, self-sacrifice, politeness and respect for authority. After a solid defensive effort, such as a double play, a pitcher will turn and bow respectfully to his infielders. A pitcher who hits a batter accidentally will tip his cap. Great emphasis is placed on wa (unity or team harmony). The Japanese are fond of the saying, “The nail that sticks up will be hammered down.” Consequently, any behavior viewed as overly individualistic or egotistical — violations of training rules, temper tantrums, moodiness, complaints to the media, attacks on umpires, salary disputes — is strongly discouraged. Team aspects of the game are valued highly: the home-run hitter is expected to make sacrifice bunts, the star pitcher to work as both a starter and a reliever. The argot of baseball changes a bit when it crosses the Pacific Ocean. Hit by pitch becomes dead ball; the game-winning home run is a sayonara home run. Also, since umpires reverse the call of balls and strikes, a full count is 2–3 (two strikes, three balls) in Japan, not 3–2 as in the US. One of Japan’s most famous baseball players, Hiromitsu Ochiai, stands out as an exception to the collective orientation of Japanese baseball. Ochiai — who led his leagues in home runs, runs batted in and batting average in both 1985 and 1986 — is the highest-paid player in the history of Japanese baseball. Yet many Japanese refer to him scornfully as a goketsu, or individual hero, and dislike him. Ochiai has skipped practices, has held-out for what is seen as an outrageously high salary and has brashly predicted that he will lead the league in home runs and batting average. Unlike previous stars such as Sadaharu Oh (who hit 868 home runs in his career), Ochiai does not fit the expected Japanese mold of the polite, deferential team man. Baseball followers may be interested in some of the following aspects of baseball in Japan, some of which reflect the collective orientation of that culture: Players are not introduced before games individually but as a team, players often make less than their managers, and foul balls hit into the stands are quietly returned to the ushers by the fans. Japan’s emphasis on team harmony, however, has not led to full acceptance of foreign players. Two foreign athletes (known as gaijin) are allowed on each professional team. Some, notably Oklahoman Randy Bass, former American major leaguer Leron Lee and home-run-hitting Cecil Fielder, have had highly successful careers in Japan. Yet almost all gaijin complain that they are treated as nothing more than outsiders. “You’re an outcast, period,” noted Warren Cromartie. “You go 0–5 and it’s Yankee go home. You go 5–5 and nobody pays attention to you” (Whiting, p. 118). Similarly, Cecil Fielder recalls that if a gaijin pitched and the team lost, “it was our fault. We didn’t do anything. But if they won, it was that they (Japanese) did it. No, no, no — we (gaijin) didn’t do it. The Japanese did it” (Muskat, p. 22). In 1986, the executive committee of Japanese baseball voted unanimously to phase out all gaijin eventually, arguing that they are overpaid and unproductive — and that Japanese baseball should be played only by Japanese. However, in 1987, former Atlanta Braves star Bob Horner signed with a Japanese team and enjoyed spectacular success and great popularity. Nevertheless, the future of gaijin in Japanese baseball remains uncertain.
Chinese families with all their worldly possessions
India's Sacred Cow: A Functionalist View
To an American tourist in
India, the Hindu prohibition against slaughtering cows may appear to be an
ignorant belief that stands in the way of progress. The Why is there such a devotion to the cow, or zebu, the large-humped species found throughout Asia and Africa? The simple explanation is that it is an integral part of Hinduism. Yet we know that many Indian people are often on the edge of starvation. Why has this practice, which appears to be manifestly dysfunctional, persisted for centuries? Economists, agronomists and social scientists working from a functionalist perspective have found that cow worship is highly functional for Indian society. For example, the zebus perform two essential tasks: plowing the fields and producing milk. If eating zebu meat were permitted, families might be tempted to slaughter their cows for immediate consumption, leaving themselves susceptible to eventual ruin. In addition, zebus produce dung, which is recovered as fertilizer and as a fuel for cooking. (American scientists are even attempting to replicate this practice to help our society meet its needs for more energy sources.) Finally, the prohibition against slaughtering cows serves the function of assisting India's poor. Untouchables (India's lowest-status group) eat zebu beef in the secrecy of their homes. Thus, the prohibition against eating beef restricts consumption by most of the population while allowing the poorest sections to obtain vitally needed nutrients otherwise missing from their diet. The tourist returns to the US with stories about the ignorant Indians. In reality, the tourist is ignorant of how functional cow worship is for Indian culture - and of how the West fails to learn from the wisdom of Indian traditions.
Conflicting CulturesThere has been a lack of total consensus on certain values in the US but the increased immigration from non-European nations during the 1990s has brought new attention to conflict. Old World habits are clashing with New World laws. The following are four examples: Cruelty to animals or Asian cuisine? In Long Beach, California, charges were dismissed against two Cambodian refugees who clubbed a puppy to death and ate it because dog-meat is often food in Cambodia. In 1996, two Iraqi immigrants - resident aliens but not citizens, aged 28 and 34 - married a pair of sisters in Lincoln, Nebraska. Shortly afterward, the men found themselves in jail because the girls were only 13 and 14, well below the state’s age of consent of 16 for marriage. They faced charges of rape and child abuse. The men were astounded to be prosecuted because their actions were in keeping with their cultural traditions. To some observers this situation seemed to support the argument that immigration must be restricted still further. However, in actuality, it should be noted that two states do allow 13-year-olds to marry. Also in 1996, two Korean women - one a Methodist missionary visiting Los Angeles, the other the wife of a Chicago doctor - died from beatings as part of anchal prayer, or church rites to exorcise personal demons. The Chicago defendants were charged with the misdemeanor of battery and received probation. National attention focused in 1996 on the case of a 19-year-old woman from Togo who told US immigration officials that she had fled her African home country in order to escape having her genitals cut off. The removal of the clitoris and the custom of infibulation, the stitching together of the labia to largely cover the vagina, is practiced in 28 African nations as a rite of passage. In these countries the practice varies widely in its prevalence and the severity of the incisions. It is largely done as a way to control women’s sexual behavior before marriage. Countries outside Africa vary in their handling of the situation. France has criminally prosecuted parents, while Canada has granted asylum to women seeking to avoid these procedures. In the US, after debate, the federal government initiated a ban, taking effect in 1997, on genital cutting. It is punishable by up to five years in prison. Furthermore, federal health agencies have been ordered to reach out to the immigrant communities and educate them about the harm of genital cutting. While many observers applaud this action, some believe that by focusing on the practice rather than handling it as an action covered by general statutes prohibiting violence against children, as France has done, the US is unfairly stereotyping Africans as people who mutilate their children. Furthermore, the federal government has not authorized any money for educational programs, which implies to some that the government is more interested in criminalizing a cultural practice than it is in assisting people in breaking with an ancient tradition. Each of these situations raises two questions of cultural relativism: When do we freely allow a different cultural practice to take place and when do we invoke sanctions to discourage, prohibit or even punish the practice of norms different from our own?
Value ConflictsIn his study of American values, Robin Williams noted that many Americans see their own group as superior to others. Along with conflict theorists, he recognized that the value of group superiority clashes with other American principles. (See Williams, 1970, American Society, New York: Knopf.) Similarly, Swedish social economist Gunnar Myrdal, in his monumental study, An American Dilemma, pointed to a contrast between the egalitarian moral precepts of the American creed (as expressed in such documents as the Declaration of Independence and the Bill of Rights) and the nation’s racist attitudes toward blacks. (See Myrdal, An American Dilemma: The Negro Problem and Modern Democracy, New York: Harper, 1944.) This discrepancy between abstract principles and people’s actual behavior was tested by Frank Westie (“The American Dilemma: An Empirical Test,” American Sociological Review, 30 (August, 1965): 527–538.) He examined the contradiction between Americans’ general commitment to egalitarian principles and specific racial prejudices by questioning a sample of 103 White adults in Indianapolis in 1957. During the first part of the interview, subjects were asked if they agreed or disagreed with a series of 10 general value statements. These statements reflected people’s support for, or opposition to, the egalitarian American creed described by Myrdal. Then, in the second part of the interview, subjects were asked to respond to hypothetical (but plausible) situations designed to test the application of these general values to the feelings each of the subjects had about blacks. The interviewers did not indicate that this was a central purpose of the study. Westie found great support for the abstract principles of the American creed. On the average, 81% of the sample endorsed such general value statements as “There should be equal opportunity,” “People should help each other in time of need” and “Recreational facilities should be available to all.” The first two received near-unanimous support; the third was approved by 63% of the sample. In contrast, however, the responses to specific value statements tended to uphold Myrdal’s theory concerning the American dilemma, the contrast between the egalitarian principles of the American creed and the ugly reality of racism in the US. On the average, only 56% of the subjects agreed with these specific statements. For example, while almost everyone felt that people should help each other in time of need, only two out of three said they would be willing to take in for a night a black family whose home had been destroyed by fire. Examination of the responses to all 10 pairs of items revealed that only 13% of the Indianapolis adults were entirely consistent in applying the general principles to hypothetical situations involving blacks. Westie’s study is not a conclusive indication of how Americans can simultaneously hold equality and group superiority as basic values. But the responses do suggest that people tend to compartmentalize their lives in a way that avoids value conflicts. They employ one set of values in situations that directly affect them, while endorsing a second set of values for other situations. Conflict theorists point to research such as that done by Westie as evidence of a lack of consensus in American society. They argue that the existence of racial prejudice and discrimination reflects a clash between the values and interests of the powerful and the oppressed.
The Skinhead CountercultureBeginning in about 1968, a new counterculture surfaced in Great Britain. The Skinheads were young people with shaved heads who often sported suspenders, tattoos and steel-toed shoes. In part, Skinhead groups emerged as vocal and sometimes violent supporters of certain British soccer teams. These young people generally came from working-class backgrounds and had little expectation of making it in mainstream society. They listened to music that extolled violence and even racism, performed by such groups as Britain’s Skrewdriver, France’s Brutal Combat and the US's Tulsa Boot Boys. Most seriously, some Skinhead groups championed racist and anti-Semitic ideologies and engaged in vandalism, violence and even murder. Immigrants from India, Pakistan and the West Indies became a common target of Skinhead attacks. (There were, however, other Skinhead groups that were explicitly antiracist). Throughout the 1970s, the Skinhead counterculture gradually spread from Britain to Europe, North America and Australia. It is difficult to measure precisely the size of this counterculture, since Skinheads do not belong to a national or international organization. Nevertheless, according to one estimate, there were 3,500 Skinheads in the US in 1993, and their numbers appeared to be growing. Skinhead groups in this country were responsible for at least 28 killings over the period 1987 to 1993. While some Skinheads around the world adopt only the distinctive dress and music associated with this counterculture, most seem to espouse white supremacy and racial hatred. In almost all the countries where Skinhead groups exist, they have committed acts of reckless violence against racial and ethnic minorities, including Jews. In the 1990s, lesbians, gay men, the homeless and people with disabilities have also become targets of Skinhead attacks. It appears that Skinheads attack those viewed as weaker to bolster their own feelings of superiority. Skinheads constitute a youthful counterculture that challenges the values of larger societies. While they claim an allegiance to history and to their (white) cultural heritage, their dress and music represent a symbolic rejection of the traditions of previous generations. Although Skinhead groups tolerate certain older adults, generally members of White supremacist and neo-Nazi organizations, this counterculture nevertheless is dominated by young males who project a tough, macho image.
Social Networks in the USSociologist Peter Marsden used the 1985 NORC General Social Survey (GSS) to gain an overview of the features of social networks. Respondents in the national interview survey were asked to name all the people with whom they discussed important matters (family, finances, health, politics, recreation and so forth) within the past six months. Subsequent questions focused on the first five names mentioned, as a concession to time constraints. The respondents were asked to describe the relationship between themselves and each of their confidants as, say, especially close or total strangers. Items describing the respondent’s relationship with each confidant (in terms of closeness, frequency of contact, duration of acquaintance and the role relations) were included, as were questions asking for the sex, race or ethnicity, education, age and religious preferences of each confidant. Among the findings was the distribution of network size: Comparatively large percentages of respondents reported that they had recently discussed important mattes with no one or with only one person. Nearly a quarter of the respondents had networks of zero or one, and thus had relatively little counseling support. Few respondents indicated that they had more than six discussion contacts; the mean and mode were three. The networks drew heavily on kinship as a source of relationships. Respondents cited a mean of 1.5 relatives, slightly more than the 1.4 nonrelatives cited. There is substantial variability in the extent to which these interpersonal networks consisted of relatives rather than nonrelatives: 30% consisted only of people having some family relation to the respondent, while nearly 20% contained no family members. The average network had a proportion of relatives of 0.55. Marsden found this comparable with the level of kin composition found in previous surveys of large populations, including network items based on intense name generators. The racial and ethnic homogeneity of the networks was pronounced; only 96 respondents (8% of those with networks of size 2 or greater) cited confidants with any racial or ethnic diversity; only 22% percent of the respondents had networks with alters of only one sex. If anything, these estimates understate the extent of homogeneity in interpersonal environments because of the high kin composition of the networks, which had many ties bridging generations and many cross-sex links to spouses, siblings, parents and children. A higher proportion of kin is associated with greater age, educational and sexual heterogeneity. If these networks had been composed only of nonrelatives, they would have been substantially less heterogeneous in these respects than the detailed findings indicated. Kin composition does, however, tend to decrease racial and ethnic heterogeneity. Overall, these descriptive figures suggest that interpersonal environments in which Americans discuss important matters are core networks, as the choice of a relatively intense name generator implies. They are small, centered on relatives, comparatively dense and homogeneous.
China and People with DisabilitiesHaving a disability is a master status found throughout the world. Sometimes its power surfaces in unusual ways. In Prior to the publicity associated with this event, sociologist C. Edward Vaughan evaluated public policy and the existing laws regarding people with disabilities in the People’s Republic of China. In 1988, China published its first five-year plan for the rehabilitation and education of people with disabilities. The Handicapped People’s Association, an organization sponsored by the Chinese government, participated in the preparatory work and discussions that led to the final document. The plan focuses on improving educational opportunities for people with disabilities and on strengthening special education programs. While the plan encourages all levels of government to enhance the employment, health, education and general welfare of people with disabilities, these policies are outlined in broad terms and lack specific goals. In 1990, China’s national government issued the Law of the People’s Republic of China on the Protection of Disabled Persons. This law was shaped, in part, through the advocacy efforts of the Disabled People’s Association. The law suggests that employers offer work opportunities to people with disabilities who pass entrance examinations. All levels of society are encouraged to offer access to people with disabilities, including access to cultural materials and transportation. To bring greater recognition to the contribution of people with disabilities, the third Sunday of every May was established as National Disabled Persons’ Day. The new legislation prohibits public officials from violating the interests or rights of people with disabilities. It outlaws violent and insulting behavior aimed at the disabled, as well as mistreatment of people with disabilities by family members or caregivers. Unfortunately, as Vaughan observes, it will be difficult for many people with disabilities to obtain justice. Few attorneys available to represent disabled people in cases arising from the 1990 law. Most people with disabilities have limited economic resources and few connections to powerful public officials.
Nature versus NurtureThe interplay between hereditary and environmental factors is evident in a fascinating study involving a young chimpanzee named Gua. (See Winthrop Kellogg and Luella Kellogg, The Ape and the Child, New York: McGraw-Hill, 1933. See also Cathy Hayes, The Ape in Our House, New York: Harper, 1951.) In 1931 Winthrop and Luella Kellogg took Gua, then 7½ months old, into their home with the intention of rearing the animal in the same way as they were rearing their nine-month-old son, Donald. The two babies lived together as companions and playmates for nine months of the experiment. Kellogg and Kellogg attempted, as far as possible, to treat, feed, and clothe their two children similarly. The Kelloggs tried to teach skills to both Donald and Gua and did not train Gua as one usually trains a household pet.
However, Gua did not surpass Donald in all areas. Donald demonstrated a much better grasp of language and use of symbols. Initially, Gua seemed to understand comments such as “Open the door” better than Donald. Kellogg and Kellogg attributed this to Gua’s greater ability to move around. In terms of speech, they foresaw that Gua would not advance beyond a few rudimentary sounds. Even at this young age, the most significant aspect distinguishing the behavior of ape from that of child involved language skills. Kellogg and Kellogg’s experiment reveals an intriguing interplay of hereditary and environmental factors. Biology seemed to limit Donald’s adaptation to walking and Gua’s potential for verbal and symbolic communication. On the other hand, socialization may well have stretched the chimpanzee’s language skills. Certainly this unusual research attests to the importance of nature in development.
Pathology of ImprisonmentAn experiment at Stanford University provided a significant critique of the impact of a total institution. Philip Zimbardo and a team of social psychologists carefully screened more than 70 volunteers for participation in a simulated prison. It is important to stress that the two dozen males selected were mature, intelligent, emotionally stable college students from middle-class homes.
The results of this experiment startled prisoners, guards and researchers alike. After only six days (rather than the intended two weeks) Zimbardo and his colleagues, aware of the ethical implications of using human subjects, had to terminate the simulation because such frightening behavior had taken place. The student guards had begun to take pleasure in cruel treatment of prisoners. Physical punishment was prohibited, but the guards created their own forms of abuse, including solitary confinement, hourly roll calls throughout the night and removal of blankets from uncooperative inmates. About a third were tyrannical and arbitrary in their use of power; and the remaining guards did not interfere with this tough approach. At the same time, the prisoners meekly accepted not only their confinement but also their mistreatment. When their requests for parole were denied, these subjects merely returned quietly to their cells, where they cried hysterically. This experiment serves as a sobering commentary on the possibility of improving prison life. Although Zimbardo argues for better training programs for prison guards, it appears that the guards themselves are prisoners of their social position as defined within the prison community and by society at large. Zimbardo’s participants were not subjected to the racism, sexual aggression and lethal violence that can be found in contemporary prisons. Therefore, it is all the more discouraging to read that four inmates were discharged prematurely because of “severe emotional responses.” Zimbardo’s research suggests that some of the problems found in prisons are inevitable and casts a disturbing shadow on hopes for reform of correctional institutions.
Deviance or SportA type of athletic contest is the subject of a clash in interpretations as to whether it constitutes sport or violence. In question are two rival organizations, the Ultimate Fighting Championship and Extreme Fighting, which mount competitions between two combatants. These competitions take place in an octagonal ring surrounded by a chain-link fence, and the only rules are no eye-gouging or biting. Chokeholds, headbutts and rabbit punches to the brain stem are legal. So are knees to the throat and elbows to the kidney. The fight is over when a doctor intervenes or when a fighter taps out, slamming a hand repeatedly on the mat. Although live audiences to such events, called extreme fighting, are limited, up to 350,000 households watched the competition between Dan “The Beast” Severn and “The Russian Bear” Oleg Taktarov at home on pay-per-view TV. Several states and cities have passed legislation banning such events and, as word spreads, more are considering that option. The stated purpose of such events is to determine which form of the martial arts, judo, kickboxing, tae kwon do, jujitsu, kung fu or another discipline — is superior. Supporters defend this type of competition and note that it pays off business-wise. They argue that football is more violent and that the critics are elitist, merely reflecting their negative opinion of the millions of people in the US who compete in the martial arts. The issues are not really new. What is defined as deviant in the realm of sport and entertainment? In different cultures boxing, bullfighting and cockfighting are variously cheered on or seen as criminal and barbaric.
Being a Hit ManHomicide violates a serious norm that is sanctioned with prison sentences and under some circumstances even with the death of the assailant. People who kill in a hot-blooded burst of passion can draw some comfort from the law, which provides lighter punishments for killings performed without premeditation or intent. But what about someone who kills repeatedly and intentionally, aware that these acts of homicide are unlawful? Ken Levi interviewed over a four-month period a self-styled hit man (referred to as Pete from Detroit) who was serving a prison sentence. Being a hit man might seem to be a life without responsibility to society’s norms, but Pete emphasizes that he is strictly governed by a contract and failure to fulfill it carries severe penalties. Pete and other hit men insist on big money because they know that less professional hired killers (such as drug addicts) who offer to work for low fees often receive a bullet for their pains. It is believed that people who would kill for so little would also require little persuasion to make them talk to the police. Therefore, his and other hit men’s reputation for charging high fees is functional; it helps them to carry out their tasks successfully and, not incidentally, to remain alive. An important way for freelance hit men to view their work as appropriate is to reframe a hit. Erving Goffman describes frames (or breaks) as portions of a given situation. Often, norm violators will dissociate themselves from a frame; a prostitute, for instance, may remain absolutely detached, her mind miles away, when having sex with a client. Even surgeons partially dissociate themselves from their patients by having the patient completely covered except for the part to be operated on; this helps them to work in a more impersonal way. Pete, the hit man interviewed by Levi, goes through a process of reframing his hits; he reveals that afterward he can rarely recall a victim’s personal features. Also, he refers to his victims as targets, not people. Even at the time of contract, he specifically requests not to be told why the contract has been let because even though the motive might justify the hit, it would make the target more of a person. Homicide is one of society’s mores. Pete knows that, but he accommodates this potentially discrediting feature of his life by emphasizing the new norms he must obey. Therefore he considers himself law-abiding, even if the laws are not those of the larger society. Similarly, he approaches the hit as just a job and thus goes as far as he can in denying his norm violation.
The Gentleman BanditAssume that an individual tries to pay his debts, loves his wife and children, believes in monogamy and fidelity, worships regularly, is a patriotic citizen and liked by everyone. Assume that individual has been a thief for over three years. Would you consider that person a deviant? Lon Perry, of Houston Texas and known as the “gentleman bandit,” committed 105 robberies over a period of three years before he turned himself in to authorities. Perry seemed to have it all — he had a lovely wife, two children, a darling grandchild and a job related to the oil business with a salary of over $50,000 a year. When an oil bust occurred in Texas, Perry lost his job at the age of 50. He couldn’t find employment. His savings began to dry up and he became depressed. At times he even thought of suicide. Finally, Perry found a solution to his dilemma — robbery, but only of specific victims who appeared to be able to afford the loss: businessmen who had expense accounts. For three years, this soft-spoken man told his family that he had a night computer software job that took him to different localities throughout Texas and neighboring states. He emphasized that they could never reach him by telephone and called in regularly to assure them of his safety. Most of the time, he was at home with his wife and grandchild during the day, but at night he was out “working.” Perry would stake out at first-class hotels, suitable candidates to rob. He would follow a person into a hotel, pretend to be a guest, travel with the person to his room and then promptly leave after a brief conversation. He would later return to the person’s room, gain entrance as a new friend and then proceed to rob the person. As the “gentleman bandit,” Perry softly explained what he was going to do and tried to reassure his victims that he was not going to harm them in any way. He even smoothed some of his victims’ hair as he tied them up face down on the bed. Most victims appeared to like him despite the robbery. One victim simply asked him to change the television channel before he left the room with the victim’s money. Another victim had only $50 in his wallet and suggested that Perry go with him to the front desk to get more money. Perry did. In a 1990 television interview, one of Perry’s victims described him as the “most courteous, polite person I have ever met.” Perry always took his victims’ business cards, recorded the amount of money he stole and solemnly vowed to himself that he would someday return the money. He never had the chance. In 1991, a man by the name of Mike Harvey was identified and accused of being the “gentleman bandit” and was taken to jail. Harvey looked so much like Perry that, when all the publicity appeared about Harvey’s arrest, Perry decided he had to turn himself in to authorities. He did and eventually received a sentence of 35 years in prison. It’s most unusual for people to feel sympathy for a person who commits a crime but even his victims felt sympathy for Perry. So … Do you consider Perry a deviant?
George Murdock’s 70 Cultural Universals
|
|